Chigwedere C* and Wilson CE
Trinity College Dublin, Ireland
*Corresponding author: Chigwedere C, Trinity College Dublin, Ireland
Submission: May 04, 2021Published: May 25, 2021
ISSN 2639-0612Volume5 Issue1
The Intolerance of Uncertainty (IOU) model of Generalized Anxiety Disorder (GAD) [1] integrates a range of psychological processes including positive beliefs about worry and a negative problem orientation, with IOU as a central vulnerability and maintaining factor. We describe the rationale behind a treatment approach applied as an adjunctive intervention in a non-inferiority trial comparing Cognitive Behavioural Psychotherapy (CBT) and Emotional-Focused Therapy (EFT); [2]. The approach incorporated amendments to the Dugas et al. [3] protocol, including targeting developmental factors and triggering events. Here we describe the clinical observation informed rationale for the amendments and how they were incorporated.
Keywords: Cognitive behavioural therapy; Generalised anxiety disorder; Intolerance of uncertainty; Worry; Schema; Attachment; Treatment; APA: American Psychiatric Association
Abbreviations: IOU: Intolerance of Uncertainty; GAD: Generalized Anxiety Disorder; CBT: Cognitive Behavioural Psychotherapy; EFT: Emotional-Focused Therapy
a. Describe the rational and hypotheses for amendments introduced in a study
b. Consider an alternative explanation of intolerance of uncertainty
c. Help to expand explanation and treatment options in the treatment of worry
Generalized Anxiety Disorder (GAD) is associated with distressing and uncontrollable worry [3]; American Psychiatric Association {APA}, [4], with symptoms including restlessness, fatigue, irritability, tension and sleep disturbance [5]; World Health Organisation {WHO}, [6,7], which cause personal and interpersonal distress and functional impairment (APA, 2000). GAD is highly co-morbid with other disorders [8] and challenging to treat [9], with only 50% of those entering treatment achieving outcomes within non-clinical ranges at discharge [10,11]. Cognitive Behavioural Therapy (CBT) has proven efficacy in the treatment of GAD. Its leading models include worry avoidance [12,13], metacognition [14,15], emotion regulation [16], and Intolerance Of Uncertainty (IoU). Based on clinical observation and theory, we added to Dugas & Robichaud’s [3] model to include a greater focus on developmental experiences, environmental triggers and perception of worry as premonitions. This adapted approach was included as an adjunctive approach in a non-inferiority trail comparing CBT and Emotion Focused Therapy (EFT). Though the model proposed by Dugas and colleagues is comprehensively described and researched, the thinking behind our amendments is not. For this reason, we will outline a three-phase maintenance explanation, with a five-module treatment structure. We will briefly outline the IoU model, followed by three concepts we think are important to our hypothesis, then an adapted worry maintenance cycle and related treatment approach.
The IoU model
The IoU model proposes that worry, a core symptom of GAD, may result directly from an
exceptional sensitivity to uncertainty. IoU is defined as “a trait of the individual, characterized by a predisposition to react negatively to an uncertain event or
situation, independent of its probability of occurrence and its
associated consequences” [17]. Due to the ubiquitous nature of
uncertainty, worry becomes diffuse, occurring in response to any
uncertain situation. For the interested reader, Dugas and Robichaud
[3] offer an extensive review of important IoU research, describing
a multifaceted explanatory and treatment approach.
They describe four main maintenance factors:
1) Intolerance of uncertainty,
2) Positive beliefs about worry,
3) Negative problem orientation and
4) Cognitive avoidance.
Treatment is offered within six modules of
1) Psychoeducation,
2) Uncertainty recognition and behavioural exposure to
real-life situations,
3) Re-evaluation of beliefs about worry as useful,
4) Problem solving,
5) Imaginal exposure to hypothetical worries and
6) Relapse prevention.
We hypothesize that developmental experiences, situational
worry, triggers, and a novel concept we have termed premonition
bias are also important.
Developmental experiences
Individuals who worry report more frequent traumas and childhood adversity, including difficult attachments, than non-anxious people [18,19]. Attachment experiences, such as enmeshment and role reversal with childhood primary caregivers [20], may be developmental precursors to interpersonal difficulties associated with worrying. They may lead to problematic internal working models of relationships (schemas) [21-24]. Young et al. [25], describe an 18-schema, 5-domain structure, which may be associated with worry [26-28]. IoU may be a function of schemas, a looming cognitive style [29] or hypervigilance for negative outcomes due to the priming effect of past negative experiences [30- 33]. It may mediate the relationship between anxious attachment and adult worry [34-37]. Furthermore, targeting attachmentrelated factors in treatment may enhance standard CBT for GAD and schema therapy has some efficacy in treating GAD. We propose that the IoU model may benefit from the inclusion of some schemafocused principles as described by Young et al. [25]. Such schemas may play an important role in the triggering of worry.
Triggering of Worry
Through associative learning, attachment-related experiences (e.g. abuse) may become Unconditioned Stimuli (UCS), paired with innocuous events (including physiological and affective states) in the learning environment [38]. Fear may be an Unconditioned Response (UCR) in this Stimulus-Stimulus (S–S) paring. However, innocuous elements before and during the event acquire the UCS’s fear-inducing qualities in a stimulus–response (S–R) association. For example, Joe’s (a composite patient) father frequently went drinking, leaving his young son alone and terrified for many hours. Later, as an adult, normal life events (e.g. his wife or children walking away from him after a disagreement) became conditioned stimuli (CS) which activated intense fear, anxiety and worry (i.e., Conditioned Response {CR}) for Joe. As such, schemas such as abandonment may represent the activation of implicit memories in current contexts. Schema activation and resultant worry, may represent a lowered threshold for perceiving implicit memory traces, which are then involuntarily retrieved by anything that resembles the CS. They represent a strong perceptual priming for the CS, triggered by anything that might vaguely resemble previously encountered negative experiences.
Such activation may strengthen the schema by creating new pairings between the CS and innocuous elements in the new environment, which elaborate and reconsolidate it. As such, the activation of worry may create more worry triggers. Schemas represent broad overgeneral implications [39]. Associated memories may be overgeneral and implicit, easily activated by traces of contextual elements that are not well discriminated from others, resulting overgeneral predictions of future events and worry behaviours. In a small study (Chigwedere, in preparation) worriers demonstrated overgeneral memory in the retrieval of past memories and future predictions. Worrying may have been an adaptive coping response for a child (like Joe above), who lacked resources for resolving the issues confronting him. However, verbal worry reduces autonomic arousal, which reinforces further worry behaviours [40-42].
Premonition bias
We propose that besides reducing arousal and avoiding distressing images, worry may have an unintended function that we have called ‘premonition bias’. Though support for this concept is currently lacking, our clinical experience is that worries are often described as having a quality of reality, as though they will eventually come true, making them believable. This might explain their fearfulness and appraisal as preparative, predictive and avoidant of negative outcomes. Worry may represent efforts to respond to negative appraisals of, and fearful predictions from current events [43]. Such appraisals and predictions are arrived at on the basis of retrieved memories of previously encountered events [44], hence the quality of reality and sense of urgency to resolve them. For example, Joe’s fear of abandonment was based on past experience and a lowered threshold for perceiving it in ambiguous situations. He was primed to perceive abandonment when his wife and children walked away, which required solutions (i.e. worrying).
Importance of the current conceptualisation
The distinction between the above explanation and the
conceptualisation of Dugas and colleagues is subtle but important.
Where they propose that what is avoided is the arousal, distress associated with fearful images of the future, we propose that it is the
triggering event in the current environment. It is avoided because
it contains reminders of memory traces of the events that gave rise
to the schema, and so activates the CR of fear, leading to worry as
solution finding. This conceptualisation potentially changes the
focus of treatment from future-focused worry to tolerance of the
ambiguous trigger in the current environment.
We propose that ambiguous events result in a worry
maintenance cycle in three phases
(1) A sense of imminent threat or fear,
(2) Apprehensive anxiety about remote, unpredictable
hypothesized outcomes, and
(3) Reinforcement, each requiring different interventions.
Though we describe them in the worry cycle below as three
distinct phases, in reality, they can be simultaneous or overlapping
and mutually activating.
The worry cycle
Fear Phase: This is a brief, three-stage, high-arousal state in
response perceived imminent threat, involving (1) orienting threat
perception, (2) appraisal of the perceived threat and (3) fright
stages.
What is it? The initial response to the trigger is a “Shto eta?”
or “What is it?” response [45]. This is a reflex, freezing response,
which we consider to be the current threat or “in the moment”
uncertainty. It is an orienting response facilitating a stop-looklisten
threat perception [46-48]. Imagine walking through a forest
associated with bear sightings. You are likely to be hypervigilant for
bears. An ambiguous rustle will make you freeze, attend and wonder
“What is it?” In the case of worry, one may be hypervigilant for the
schema. Like the rustling, ambiguous events activate the freeze and
attend response, during which sensory and attentional resources
are directed towards evaluation of the source of the threat stimulus.
Appraisal/meaning making: We propose that “What is it?” is
immediately followed by a rapid appraisal stage. For example, “What
is it?” is rapidly followed by an awareness of “I don’t know what it is,”
(i.e. uncertainty) then “What if it’s a threat/bear!” (i.e. explanatory
appraisals retrieved from stored information as appraisal of the
rustling). This involves the retrieval of autobiographical memories,
encoded in terms of their general sense as implicational, schematic
aspects [49]. Such schemas give an overgeneral meaning or
appraisal to the current context (i.e. rustle = bear). The earlier
case of Joe offers an example of this process (i.e. walking away =
abandonment).
Such appraisals and memories are involuntarily, directly and
rapidly retrieved by ambiguous cues or memory reminders within
environments, not through voluntary, indirect and effortful recall
[50-52]. They will be accompanied by sensory and emotional
memory fragments, similar to trauma memories [53], but in worry,
processing may stop at the overgeneral, rather than the event
specific knowledge level [54-56]. Such overgeneral memories will
be accompanied by high arousal and emotionality. “Fear Phase”. The
rapid negative appraisal of ambiguity results in a sense of imminent
threat (e.g. “I’m…being abandoned/…under threat”), because ‘it
is better to be safe than sorry’. Arousal is increased, uncertainty
is experienced as frightening and intolerable, which motivates
prediction of outcome scenarios. At this stage, one reaches the peak
of a brief “Fear Phase”. which started with a sense of not knowing
(i.e. what is it?) and is associated with intense amygdala activation
[57].
Anxiety “Fear Phase” before: This is a prolonged prolonged,
apprehensive state of lower intensity arousal than the fear phase,
comprising (1) flight into worry and (2) premonition bias stages.
Flight into worry: Fear motivates safety-seeking and activates
a flight phase. However, ambiguous triggers, overgeneral appraisals
and high uncertainty preclude the identification of behavioural
safety-measures. Overgeneral memories and their appraisals are
accessed to facilitate solution-finding and plan goals, which will
be abstract and verbal and reduce autonomic arousal [58]. This
leads to cyclical, verbal worry in a quest for certainty. Indeed, such
overgeneral encoding and retrieval have been associated with both
poor problem-solving [59-61] and overgeneral imaginings of the
future, which may not be uncommon in worry (Chigwedere, in
preparation).
Premonition bias: Worry may be a freeze-fright-flight-fight
response to avoid the trigger event. Uncertainty may be experienced
differently at each stage, though its main driver may be schema
activation in the current environment. The worry phase may
represent flight and fight responses. One flees “into worry” avoid
the trigger and searches for solutions as fighting. Unfortunately,
the identified hypothetical solutions are perceived as highly
likely to occur at an unknown future point (i.e. premonition bias),
representing non-specific and unpredictable threats. Consequently,
the individual experiences anxiety, an anticipatory response to
uncertain, unpredictable threat, associated with activation of the
bed nucleus of the stria terminalis [62-64]. Premonition bias may
partially account for positive worry beliefs. As such, positive beliefs
about worry may initially be responses to simply experiencing
worry [65], which strengthen in believability to become part of the
maintenance cycle of worry.
Reinforcement Phase
Worry behaviours: Worry predictions and premonition
bias increase uncertainty and activate overt and covert worry
behaviours. Covert responses may include mental actions such
as distraction, thinking positively, thinking of likely scenarios and
solution finding. Overt worry behaviours may include reassurance
seeking, behavioural avoidance and checking.
Reinforcement: Worry behaviours result in reduced arousal
and a perception of ‘near miss’ (i.e. “If I had not done safetybehaviours,
a negative outcome really would have come true”).
Consequently, the worry behaviours are reinforced, and the near
miss strengthens the belief in the schema. Figure 1 diagrammatically
represents this hypothesised worry cycle.
Figure 1: Three-phase model of worry maintenance.
The treatment
As already stated, we will mostly focus on describing the adjunctive, novel components of the treatment. The Dugas & Robichaud [3] approach was followed in the Timulak et al. [2] study. However, unlike Dugas and Robichaud, in that study we introduced imaginal exposure to worry earlier in the treatment, as reported by others [66]. It is worth noting that the approach described here was successfully applied as a standalone treatment in a small case series (Chigwedere & Moran, under review). The approach was designed to be used flexibly in a modular system.
Module 1: Socialisation to the model, goal setting and self-monitoring: This module follows a thorough assessment. The main objective is socialising the patient to the model and psychoeducation about IoU. The therapist offers the explanatory rationale for the maintenance of worry, the concept of uncertainty and the synergy of cognitions, affect and behaviours. The dimensionality of worry is reinforced using metaphors, such as the allergy metaphor. Principles of imaginal exposure are introduced. In-session practice is used to demonstrate the cognitive avoidance function of worry, demonstrating how it avoids ambiguous, in-themoment triggers leading to increased uncertainty and worry. Most patients find a description of worry using simple explanations of neural structures helpful, particularly the idea that worry may reduce neural connectivity and the integrity of mechanisms such as the amygdala and uncinate fasciculus, which may be reversible with mindfulness and CBT [67]. Hypothetical worrying is described as an apprehensive anxiety response, with a simple description of prefrontal cortex-limbic system connectivity. It is described to patients in simple terms as a cognitive activity, whose function is akin to a covert safety-behaviour. The concept of premonition bias is demonstrated by simply asking the patient to describe the quality of their worrying thoughts, whether they feel like “just your own fears or realities that could actually come true.”
Depending on therapist skill, as early as the socialisation exercise at the end of the assessment, the difference between hypothetical worry and focusing on the real problem (i.e. the trigger and the uncertainty) is demonstrated. Some patients have been able to begin to practice ‘sitting with’ internally or externally activated sensory images of the trigger and associated uncertainty. Self-monitoring is practiced in tandem with practicing identifying and tolerating uncertainty and its triggers. At the start of the first session following assessment, therapist and patient collaboratively identify specific end-of-treatment goals and each session ends with a specific homework task. Depending on therapist skill and patient resources, this module could be up to 3 sessions. Worry monitoring uses a novel Worry/Uncertainty Monitoring Diary, which distinguishes current uncertainty triggers from their hypothetical worry consequences.
Module 2: Self-monitoring and worry recognition training: While socialization, self-monitoring and tolerance training can start as early as the end of the assessment session, Module 2 specifically targets intolerance of uncertainty and increasing worry awareness. As some patients can find the imaginal exposure worry scripts [3] too distressing, in the Timulak et al. [2] study, we introduced exposure to ‘simple still images’ without scripts, which increase in detail and complexity with habituation, until patients are able to tolerate complex scripted scenes. Still images merely involve patients describing their feared scene and stopping when they reach a ‘hot-spot’ (i.e., an image that raises notable anxiety). At this point they are guided to hold this image, ‘like looking at a photograph’, while rating their anxiety to the point of habituation. In the case series (Chigwedere & Moran, under review), we only referred to imaginal exposure to the future as a way of demonstrating the effects of worry.
Uncertainty Tolerance Training (UTT)
We have called sitting with uncertainty triggers and associated uncertainty to the point of tolerance UTT. This is because the objective is not exposure and habituation but increased tolerance and acceptance of the sense of not knowing, not unlike inhibitory learning [35]. In each session, patient and therapist collaboratively identify a recent worry event and the environmental trigger, with which the patient imaginally engages non-judgmentally, resisting the urge ‘flee into a quest for certainty’ by worrying. Specific events are identified and engaged with as homework. Once adept at holding the image, patients are introduced to the idea of an uncertainty statement, making the uncertainty an explicit component of the UTT. As the patient becomes adept at identifying worry triggers and uncertainty, they are encouraged to increasingly practice with live events. We hypothesize that tolerance of the uncertainty triggers in the current environment reduces fear and the flight into a ‘quest for certainty’ (i.e., worry). Some patients who have practiced both imaginal exposure and UTT, have described imaginal exposure as bearing similarities to worry, which is hypothetical and avoids the current uncertainty. As patients become more skilled with UTT, a problem-solving component is introduced. This involves therapists and patients collaboratively identifying what is known, (e.g. “I know my wife is walking away”) and not known (e.g. “I don’t know what she is thinking”). The therapist then asks “You know… but you don’t know …. What options do you have, as you look at this?” The patient then practices learning to problem solve the uncertain event, not a future hypothesis.
Module 3: Contextualizing intolerance of uncertainty and worry: This module aims to help clients to hypothesize the possible developmental contexts of their worry. The hypothesis is that many important cognitions have roots in developmental experiences. Cognitive work has a function of testing developmentally as tests of developmentally acquired schema-based messages, identifying and developing healthier alternatives. Individuals with GAD have a tendency to worry and ruminate, so we encourage the use imagery and experiential methods. The aim of this module is to contextualise the worry, taking advantage of the preceding work. To do this, current or worry events are first engaged with as in UTT, identifying cognitive and affective states. Affect is then engaged with and traced back, using it as a link to childhood memories of similar experiences. Once such memories are identified, imagery rescripting work is employed, and collaboratively linked back to current worry, either experientially through rescripting or in reflection. Besides rescripting developmental events, we have found this to be a powerful way of identifying different levels of emotionladen cognitions, including their helpful alternatives (Chigwedere & Moran, in preparation). Learning from rescripting work is a useful way of identifying schemas or core beliefs and to develop hypotheses to be tested and challenged through standard CBT techniques (e.g. behavioural experiments, surveys and exposure). We have also developed a diary sheet to help patients to identify ways in which they may have reinforced the schema/core beliefs through the years.
Module 4: Problem-solving: If worry is merely a dysfunctional attempt at problem-solving due to lack of a credible alternative, offering a helpful alternative may reduce reliance on the dysfunctional one. Problem-solving has been described as a response to obstacles to life goals, which are for a time, insurmountable with customary methods [29]. It has been used as a standalone or component therapeutic technique. Therapists engage in problem solving using the 6 stages described by Dugas and Robichaud [3]. These include 1) problem definition, 2) choosing a target problem, 3) agreeing on goals, 4) solution generation, 5) choosing a solution, 6) establishing the necessary steps to implement the solution, 7) solution implementation...” 8) evaluation of the effectiveness of the solution. This is a more thorough skills training approach than that used as a component of UTT in Module 2.
Module 5: Mindfulness: UTT may be reminiscent of ‘mindfulness’, In mindfuless, one engages non-judgmentally with a neutral anchor that is not problem specific (e.g. the breath), with profound impacts. However, a neutral anchor may not engage the schema. The UTT ‘anchor’ (i.e. the image and uncertainty statement) may be a schema-related CS. In non-judgemental engagement with the CS one may learn that it can be tolerated and reappraised. It is not necessary to continue such focused daily UTT or imaginal exposure ad infinitum, making mindfulness an appropriate maintenance and relapse prevention technique. Thus, once GAD and worry symptom improvement are achieved, standard mindfulness is introduced.
We describe the IoU model but with important adaptations, including a focus on current triggers of uncertainty rather than the future focused worry, as well as making the uncertainty explicit. We offer an alternative rationale for worry as a premonition bias, due to its association with schemas and developmental events. We propose a three-phase description of the worry process, with the environmental trigger as an important target for intervention. Our treatment approach incorporates this hypothesis within the standard, evidence-based approach but offering an alternative explanation, which potentially brings new treatment possibilities. The contextual module potentially offers each individual an explanation of the why and how of their own worry. Though much of what we have proposed is based on clinical observation and literature, it has some evidence of effectiveness through its application in a research study and a case series. This makes this description paper important for others who may wish to apply this approach clinically as either a standalone or adjunctive approach.
© 2021 Chigwedere C, This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License , which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and build upon your work non-commercially.